Advertisement

Description of Bird Behavior for Comparative Purposes

  • Edward H. Miller
Chapter
Part of the Current Ornithology book series (CUOR, volume 5)

Abstract

Ethology arose as a discipline within zoology, with distinctly different aims and methods from psychology (Tinbergen, 1963). From its inception, ethology embraced phylogenesis, inheritance, “survival value,” and adaptation—research areas that relied on comparative observations and inductive reasoning (Darwin, 1859, 1872). Our understanding of animal behavior has increased through many fine descriptive studies and because ethology has become more rigorous, quantitative, experimental, and interdisciplinary. Ethology needs a richness of research philosophies and approaches because animal behavior, however it is viewed or defined, is complex, dynamic, and influenced by many internal and external factors with enormously diverse time courses. A comprehensive understanding of behavioral attributes of bird feeding, for example, demands knowledge of motor patterns, feeding mechanisms, diet, development, sensory biology, behavioral ecology, food choice, functional morphology, and other subjects. At the other extreme, progress toward understanding broad transspecific behavioral trends, or toward better standardized descriptive methods in ethology, depends on a knowledge of diverse taxa (Schleidt et al., 1984; Leonard and Lukowiak, 1984).

Keywords

Motor Pattern Behavioral Category Herring Gull Great Egret Eastern Bluebird 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Afton, A. D., 1979, Time budget of breeding Northern Shovelers, Wilson Bull. 91:42–49.Google Scholar
  2. Andersson, M., 1974, Temporal graphical analysis of behaviour sequences, Behaviour 51:38–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Andersson, M., 1976, Social behaviour and communication in the Great Skua, Behaviour 58:40–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Anonymous, 1986, Auk 103:681.Google Scholar
  5. Baerends, G. P., Drent, R. H., Glas, P., and Groenewold, H., 1970, An ethological analysis of incubation behaviour in the Herring Gull, Behaviour (Suppl.) 17:135–235.Google Scholar
  6. Baker, M. C., 1973, Stochastic properties of the foraging behavior of six species of migratory shorebirds, Behaviour 45:242–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Barlow, G. W., 1968, Ethological units of behavior, in: The Central Nervous System and Fish Behavior (D. Ingle, ed.), University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 217–232.Google Scholar
  8. Barlow, G. W., 1977, Modal action patterns, in: How Animals Communicate (T. A. Sebeok, ed.), Indiana University Press, Indianapolis, pp. 98–134.Google Scholar
  9. Barlow, J. C., and Rice, J. C, 1977, Aspects of the comparative behavior of Red-eyed and Philadelphia Vireos, Can. J. Zool. 55:528–542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Beer, C. G., 1980, The communication behavior of gulls and other seabirds, in: Behavior of Marine Animals, Volume 4 (J. Burger, B. L. Olla, and H. E. Winn, eds.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 169–205.Google Scholar
  11. Bekoff, M., 1977, Quantitative studies of three areas of classical ethology: Social dominance, behavioral taxonomy, and behavioral variability, in: Quantitative Methods in the Study of Animal Behavior (B. A. Hazlett, ed.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 1–46.Google Scholar
  12. Binkley, S., Adler, K., and Taylor, D. H., 1973, Two methods for using period length to study rhythmic phenomena, J. Comp. Physiol. 83:63–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Bird, J., Riska, B., and Sokal, R. R., 1981, Geographic variation in variability of Pemphigus populicaulis, Syst. Zool. 30:58–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bond, A. B., Barlow, G. W., and Rogers, W., 1985, Two modal action patterns with a continuous temporal distribution, Z. Tierpsychol. 68:326–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Bookstaber, R., and Langsam, J., 1985, On the optimality of coarse behavior rules, J. Theor. Biol. 116:161–193.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Brent, S. B., 1984, Psychological and Social Structures, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ.Google Scholar
  17. Bruggers, R., and Jackson, W. B., 1981, Age-related differences in reproductive behavior of male Mandarin Ducks, Bird Study 28:107–114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Burger, A. E., 1980, An analysis of the displays of Lesser Sheathbills Chionis minor, Z. Tierpsychol. 52:381–396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Caraco, T., 1982, Flock size and the organization of behavioral sequences in juncos, Condor 84:58–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Colgan, P. W. (ed.), 1978, Quantitative Ethology, Wiley-Interscience, New York.Google Scholar
  21. Craig, J. L., 1977, The behaviour of the Pukeko, Porphyrio porphyrio melanotus, N.Z. J.Zool. 4:413–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Crook, J. H., 1964, The evolution of social organisation and visual communication in the weaver birds (Ploceinae), Behav. Suppl. 10.Google Scholar
  23. Dane, B., Walcott, C., and Drury, W. H., 1959, The form and duration of the display actions of the Goldeneye (Bucephala clangula), Behaviour 14:265–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Darwin, C., 1859, The Origin of Species, Appleton, New York.Google Scholar
  25. Darwin, C., 1872, The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, Appleton, New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Davies, W. G., 1978, Cluster analysis applied to the classification of postures in the Chilean Flamingo (Phoenicopterus chilensis), Anim. Behav. 26:381–388.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Dawkins, M., and Dawkins, R., 1974, Some descriptive and explanatory stochastic models of decision-making, in: Motivational Control Systems Analysis (D. J. McFarland, ed.), Academic Press, London, pp. 119–168.Google Scholar
  28. Dawkins, M. S., 1983, The organisation of motor patterns, in: Animal Behaviour, Volume 1: Causes and Effects (T. R. Halliday and P. J. B. Slater, eds.), Freeman, San Francisco, pp. 75–99.Google Scholar
  29. Dawkins, M. S., 1986, Unravelling Animal Behaviour, Longman, Harlow, U.K.Google Scholar
  30. Dawkins, R., 1976, Hierarchical organisation: A candidate principle for ethology, in: Growing Points in Ethology (P. P. G. Bateson and R. A. Hinde, eds.), Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K., pp. 7–54.Google Scholar
  31. Dawkins, R., and Dawkins, M., 1973, Decisions and the uncertainty of behaviour, Behaviour 45:83–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Delius, J. D., 1969, A stochastic analysis of the maintenance behaviour of Skylarks, Behaviour 33:137–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Dörrscheidt, G. J., and Beck, L., 1975, Advanced methods for evaluating characteristic parameters (τ, α, ρ) of circadian rhythms, J. Math. Biol. 2:107–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Drummond, H., 1981, The nature and description of behavior patterns, in Perspectives in Ethology, Volume 4 (P. P. G. Bateson and P. H. Klopfer, eds.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 1–33.Google Scholar
  35. Drummond, H., 1985, Towards a standard ethogram: Do ethologists really want one?, Z. Tierpsychol. 68:338–339.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Fagen, R. M., 1978, Repertoire analysis, in: Quantitative Ethology (P. W. Colgan, ed.), Wiley-Interscience, New York, pp. 25–42.Google Scholar
  37. Fagen, R. M., and Young, D. Y., 1978, Temporal patterns of behavior: Durations, intervals, latencies, and sequences, in: Quantitative Ethology (P. W. Colgan, ed.), Wiley-Interscience, New York, pp. 79–114.Google Scholar
  38. Fjeldsa, J., 1983, Social behaviour and displays of the Hoary-headed Grebe Poliocephalus poliocephalus, Emu 83:129–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Frith, C. B., and Frith, D. W., 1981, Displays of Lawės’s Parotia Parotia lawesii (Paradisaeidae), with reference to those of congeneric species and their evolution, Emu 81:227–238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Glutz von Blotzheim, U. N., Bauer, K. M., and Bezzel, E. (eds.), 1975, Handbuch der Vögel Mitteleuropas, Band 6: Charadriiformes (Vol. 1), Akademische Verlagsgesellschaften, Wiesbaden, F. R. G.Google Scholar
  41. Golani, I., 1976, Homeostatic motor processes in mammalian interactions: A choreography of display, in: Perspectives in Ethology, Volume 2 (P. P. G. Bateson and P. H. Klopfer, eds.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 69–134.Google Scholar
  42. Golani, L, Wolgin, D. L., and Teitelbaum, P., 1979, A proposed natural geometry of recovery from akinesia in the lateral hypothalamic rat, Brain Res. 164:237–267.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Gordon, D. M., 1985, Do we need more ethograms?, Z. Tierpsychol. 68:340–342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Hailman, J. P., 1976, Review of Function and Evolution in Behaviour (G. Baerends, C. Beer, and A. Manning, eds.), Science 193:993–994.Google Scholar
  45. Hailman, J. P., 1977a, Optical Signals: Animal Communication and Light, Indiana University Press, Bloomington.Google Scholar
  46. Hailman, J. P., 1977b, Communication by reflected light, in: How Animals Communicate (T. A. Sebeok, ed.), Indiana University Press, Indianapolis, pp. 184–210.Google Scholar
  47. Hamilton, W. J. III, 1965, Sun-oriented display of the Annas Hummingbird, Wilson Bull. 77:38–43.Google Scholar
  48. Hazlett, B. C. (ed.), 1977, Quantitative Methods in the Study of Animal Behavior, Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar
  49. Heiligenberg, W., 1974, Processes governing behavioral states of readiness, in: Advances in the Study of Behavior, Volume 5 (D. S. Lehrman, J. S. Rosenblatt, R. A. Hinde, and E. Shaw, eds.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 173–200.Google Scholar
  50. Helbing, G. L., 1977, Maintenance activities of the Black Oystercatcher, Haematopus bachmani Audubon, in northwestern California, M.S. thesis, Humboldt State University, Arcata, California.Google Scholar
  51. Hinde, R. A., and Stevenson, J. G., 1969, Sequences of behavior, in: Advances in the Study of Behavior, Volume 2 (D. S. Lehrman, R. A. Hinde, and E. Shaw, eds.), Academic Press, New York, pp. 267–296.Google Scholar
  52. Hummel, T. J., and Sligo, J. R., 1971, Empirical comparison of univariate and multivariate analysis of variance procedures, Psychol. Bull. 76:49–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. James, F. C., and McCulloch, C. E., 1985, Data analysis and the design of experiments in ornithology, in: Current Ornithology, Volume 2 (R. F. Johnston, ed.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 1–63.Google Scholar
  54. Janzen, D. M., 1986, The future of tropical ecology, Annu. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 17:305–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Jehl, J. R., Jr., 1975, Pluvianellus socialis: Biology, ecology, and relationships of an enigmatic Patagonian shorebird, Trans. San Diego Soc. Nat. Hist. 18:25–74.Google Scholar
  56. Kahl, M. P., 1972, Comparative ethology of the Ciconiidae. 2. The Adjutant Storks, Leptoptilos dubius (Gmelin) and L. javanicus (Horsfield), Ardea 60:97–111.Google Scholar
  57. Krieg, D. C., 1971, The behavioral patterns of the Eastern Bluebird, N.Y. State Mus. Sci. Serv. Bull. 415.Google Scholar
  58. Lehner, P. N., 1979, Handbook of Ethological Methods, Garland STPM Press, New York.Google Scholar
  59. Lemnell, P. A., 1978, Social behaviour of the Great Snipe Capella media at the arena display, Omis Scand. 9:1 6–163.Google Scholar
  60. Leonard, J. L., and Lukowiak, K., 1984, An ethogram of the sea slug, Navanax inermis (Gastropoda; Opisthobranchia), Z. Tierpsychol. 65:327–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Leonard, J. L., and Lukowiak, K., 1985, The standard ethogram: A two-edged sword?, Z. Tierpsychol. 68:335–337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Lewontin, R. C., Rose, S., and Kamin, L. J., 1984, Not in Our Genes: Biology, Ideology, and Human Nature, Pantheon, New York.Google Scholar
  63. Lind, H., 1961, Studies on the Behaviour of the Black-tailed Godwit (Limosa limosa (L.)), Munksgaard, Copenhagen.Google Scholar
  64. Machlis, L., 1977, An analysis of the temporal patterning of pecking in chicks, Behaviour 63:1–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Machlis, L., Dodd, P. W. D., and Fentress, J. C., 1985, The pooling fallacy: Problems arising when individuals contribute more than one observation to the data set, Z. Tierpsychol. 68:201–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Martin, P., and Bateson, P., 1986, Measuring Behaviour: An introductory Guide, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, U.K.Google Scholar
  67. McFarland, D. (ed.), 1981, The Oxford Companion to Animal Behaviour, Oxford University Press, Oxford, U.K.Google Scholar
  68. McFarland, D. J., and Baher, E., 1968, Factors affecting feather posture in the Barbary Dove, Anim. Behav. 16:171–177.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. McKearney, J. W., 1977, Asking questions about behavior, Persp. Biol. Med. 21:109–119.Google Scholar
  70. McKinney, F., 1965, The comfort movements of Anatidae, Behaviour 25:120–220.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. McKinney, F., 1975, The evolution of duck displays, in: Function and Evolution in Behaviour (G. Baerends, C. Beer, and A. Manning, eds.), Clarendon Press, Oxford, U.K., pp. 331–357.Google Scholar
  72. Merton, D. V., Morris, R. B., and Atkinson, I. A. E., 1984, Lek behaviour in a parrot: The Kakapo Strigops habroptilus of New Zealand, Ibis 126:277–283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Miller, E. H., 1975, Walrus ethology. I. The social role of tusks and applications of multidimensional scaling, Can. J. Zool. 53:590–613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Miller, E. H., 1984, Communication in breeding shorebirds, in: Shorebirds: Breeding Behavior and Populations (Behavior of Marine Animals, Volume 5) (J. Burger and B. Olla, eds.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 169–241.Google Scholar
  75. Miller, E. H., 1985, Museum collections and the study of animal social behaviour, in: Museum Collections: Their Roles and Future in Biological Research (E. H. Miller, ed.), Occasional Paper No. 25, British Columbia Provincial Museum, Victoria, BC, Canada, pp. 139–162.Google Scholar
  76. Miller, R.S., and Stephen, W. J. D., 1966, Spatial relationships in flocks of Sandhill Cranes (Grus canadensis), Ecology 47:323–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Mock, D. W., 1975, Social behavior of the Boat-billed Heron, Living Bird 14:185–214.Google Scholar
  78. Mock, D. W., 1978, Pair-formation displays of the Great Egret, Condor 80:159–172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Mock, D. W., 1980, Communication strategies of Great Blue Herons and Great Egrets, Behaviour 72:156–170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Moltz, H., 1965, Contemporary instinct theory and the fixed action pattern, Psychol. Rev. 72:27–47.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Morgan, B. J. T., Simpson, M. J. A., Hanby, J. P., and Hall-Craggs, J., 1976, Visualizing interaction and sequential data in animal behaviour: Theory and application of cluster-analysis methods, Behaviour 55:1–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Morris, D., 1956, The feather postures of birds and the problem of the origin of social signals, Behaviour 9:75–113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Myers, J. P., Cardillo, R. F., and Culbertson, M. A., 1984, VIREO: Visual resources for ornithology, Am. Birds 38:267–277.Google Scholar
  84. Myers, J. P., Cardillo, R. F., and Gill, F. B., 1986, VIREO: Procedures and services for the ornithological community, Condor 88:115–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Nichols, J. D., Tomlinson, R. E., and Waggerman, G., 1986, Estimating nest detection probabilities for White-winged Dove nest transects in Tamaulipas, Mexico, Auk 103:825–828.Google Scholar
  86. Nol, E., 1984, Reproductive strategies in the oystercatchers (Aves: Haematopodidae), Ph.D. Thesis, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ont., Canada.Google Scholar
  87. Nol, E., 1985, Sex roles in the American Oystercatcher, Behaviour 95:232–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Norberg, R. A., 1986, Treecreeper climbing: Mechanics, energetics, and structural adaptations, Ornis Scand. 17:191–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Oehme, H., 1985, Uber die Flugelbnewegung der Vogel im Schnellen Streckenflug, Milu (Berl.) 6:137–156.Google Scholar
  90. Pielou, E. C., 1977, Mathematical Ecology, John Wiley, New York.Google Scholar
  91. Pinkowski, B. C., 1979, Time budget and incubation rhythm of the Eastern Bluebird, Am. Midl. Nat. 101:427–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Post, W., 1974, Functional analysis of space-related behavior in the Seaside Sparrow, Ecology 55:564–575.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Potts, K. J., 1976, Comfort movements of the Kea, Nestor notabilis (Psittaciformes: Nestoridae), Notornis 23:302–309.Google Scholar
  94. Powers, W. T., 1973, Feedback: Beyond behaviorism, Science 179:351–356.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Purdy, M. A., 1985, Parental behaviour and role differentiation in the Black Oystercatcher Haematopus bachmani, M.Sc. Thesis, University of Victoria, Victoria, BC, Canada.Google Scholar
  96. Radesater, T., 1974, Form and sequential associations between the triumph ceremony and other behaviour patterns in the Canada Goose Branta canadensis L., Ornis Scand. 5:87–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Schleidt, W. M., 1974, How “fixed” is the fixed action pattern? Z. Tierpsychol 36:184–211.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Schleidt, W. M., 1985, In defense of standard ethograms, Z. Tierpsychol. 68:343–345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Schleidt, W. M., Yakalis, G., Donnelly, M., and McGary, J., 1984, A proposal for a standard ethogram, exemplified by an ethogram of the Bluebreasted Quail (Coturnix chinensis), Z. Tierpsychol. 64:193–220.Google Scholar
  100. Schnell, G. D., and Woods, B. L., 1983, Application of numerical taxonomic techniques in the study of behavior, in: Numerical Taxonomy: Proceedings of a NATO Advanced Study institute (J. Felsenstein, ed.), NATO Adv. Study Inst. Ser. G (Ecological Sciences), No. 1, Springer-Verlag, New York, pp. 562–581.Google Scholar
  101. Schnell, G. D., Watt, D. J., and Douglas, M. E., 1985, Statistical comparison of proximity matrices: Applications in animal behaviour, Anim. Behav. 33:239–253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Schulz, H., 1986, Agonistisches Verhalten, Territorialverhalten und Balz der Zwergtrappe (Tetrax tetrax), J. Orn. 127:125–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  103. Shepard, R. N., 1974, Representation of structure in similarity data: Problems and prospects, Psychometrika 39:373–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Short, L. L., Jr., 1984, Woodpeckers of the World. Delaware Museum of Natural History, Greenville.Google Scholar
  105. Short, R., and Horn, J., 1984, Some notes on factor analysis of behavioral data, Behaviour 90:203–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Slater, P. J. B., 1973, Describing sequences of behavior, in: Perspectives in Ethology (P. P. G. Bateson and P. H. Klopfer, eds.), Plenum Press, New York, pp. 131–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Slater, P. J. B., 1974a, Bouts and gaps in the behaviour of Zebra Finches, with special reference to preening, Rev. Comp. Anim. 8:47–61.Google Scholar
  108. Slater, P. J. B., 1974b, A reassessment of ethology, in: The Biology of Brains (W. B., Broughton, ed.), Blackwell Scientific, London, pp. 89–113.Google Scholar
  109. Slater, P. J. B., 1975, Temporal patterning and the causation of bird behaviour, in: Neural and Endocrine Aspects of Behaviour in Birds (P. Wright, P. G. Caryl, and D. M. Vowles, eds.), Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp. 11–33.Google Scholar
  110. Slater, P. J. B., 1978, Data collection, in: Quantitative Ethology (P. W. Colgan, ed.), Wiley-Interscience, New York, pp. 7–24.Google Scholar
  111. Slater, P. J. B., and Lester, N. P., 1982, Minimising errors in splitting behaviour into bouts, Behaviour 79:153–161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  112. Smith, W. J., 1971, Behavior of Muscisaxicola and related genera. Bull. Mus. Comp. Zool. 141:233–268.Google Scholar
  113. Smith, W. J., 1977, The Behavior of Communicating: An Ethological Approach, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
  114. Smith, W. J., 1985, Consistency and change in communication, in: The Development of Expressive Behavior: Biology-Environment Interactions (G. Zivin, ed.), Academic Press, New York. pp. 51–76.Google Scholar
  115. Sneath, P. H. A., and Sokal, R. R., 1973, Numerical Taxonomy: The Principles and Practice of Numerical Classification, Freeman, San Francisco.Google Scholar
  116. Sokal, R. R., and Braumann, C. A., 1980, Significance tests for coefficients of variation and variability profiles, Syst. Zool. 29:50–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  117. Sokal, R. R., and Rohlf, F. J., 1981, Biometry: The Principles and Practice of Statistics in Biological Research, 2nd ed., Freeman, San Francisco.Google Scholar
  118. Sokolove, P. G., and Bushell, W. N., 1978, The chi-square periodogram: Its utility for analysis of circadian rhythms, J. Theor. Biol. 72:131–160.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  119. Sparks, J. H., 1964, Flock structure of the Red Avadavat with particular reference to clumping and allopreening, Anim. Behav. 12:125–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. Standen, P. J., 1980, The social display of the Chilean Teal Anas flavirostris flavirostris, J. Zool. 191:293–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  121. Taylor, P. S., 1973, Breeding behavior of the Snowy Owl, Living Bird 12:137–154.Google Scholar
  122. Tinbergen, N., 1948, Social releasers and the experimental method required for their study, Wilson Bull. 60:6–51.Google Scholar
  123. Tinbergen, N., 1963, On aims and methods of ethology, Z. Tierpsychol. 20:410–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  124. Todt, D., and Fiebelkorn, A., 1980, Displays, timing and function of wing movement accompanying antiphonal duets of Cichladusa guttata, Behaviour 72:82–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Van der Kloot, W., and Morse, M. J., 1975, A stochastic analysis of the display behavior of the Red-breasted Merganser (Mergus serrator), Behaviour 54:181–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  126. Van der Steen, W. J., and Ter Maat, A., 1979, Theoretical studies on animal orientation. I. Methodological appraisal of classifications, J. Theor. Biol. 79:223–234.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  127. Van Iersel, J. J. A., and Bol, A. C. A., 1958, Preening of two tern species: A study on displacement activities, Behaviour 13:1–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  128. Van Rhijn, J. C, 1977, The patterning of preening and other comfort behaviour in a Herring Gull, Behaviour 63:71–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  129. Van Rhijn, J. G., 1981, Units of behaviour in the Black-headed Gull, Larus ridibundus L., Anim. Behav., 29:586–597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  130. Walsberg, G. E., Brood size and the use of time and energy by the Phainopepla, Ecology 59:147–153.Google Scholar
  131. Wiley, R. H., 1973, The strut display of male Sage Grouse: A “fixed” action pattern, Behaviour 47:129–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  132. Wiley, R. H., 1975, Multidimensional variation in an avian display: Implications for social communication, Science 190:482–483.Google Scholar
  133. Willig, M. R., Owen, R. D., and Colbert, R. L., 1986, Assessment of morphometric variation in natural populations: The inadequacy of the univariate approach, Syst. Zool. 35:195–203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  134. Wilson, R. H., 1974, Agonistic postures and latency to the first interaction during initial pair encounters in the Red Jungle Fowl, Gallus gallus, Anim. Behav. 22:75–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  135. Wolf, L. L., and Hainsworth, F. R., 1971, Time and energy budgets of territorial hummingbirds, Ecology 52:980–988.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  136. Wright, S., 1978, Evolution and the Genetics of Populations, Volume 4; Variability Within and Among Natural Populations, University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  137. Yablokov, A. V., 1974, Variability of Mammals, Amerind Publishing Co., New Delhi.Google Scholar
  138. Zahavi, A., 1971, The social behaviour of the White Wagtail Motacilla alba alba wintering in Israel, Ibis 113:203–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  139. Zweers, G. A., 1974, Structure, movement and myography of the feeding apparatus of the Mallard (Anas platyrhynchos L.), Neth. J. Zool 24:323–467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  140. Zweers, G. A., 1982, Drinking of the Pigeon (Columbia livia L.), Behaviour 80:274–317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Plenum Press, New York 1988

Authors and Affiliations

  • Edward H. Miller
    • 1
    • 2
  1. 1.Department of BiologyUniversity of VictoriaVictoriaCanada
  2. 2.Collections ProgramRoyal British Columbia MuseumVictoriaCanada

Personalised recommendations